Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Apraxia and Alzheimer’s Disease: Review and Perspectives

  • Review
  • Published:
Neuropsychology Review Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Apraxia is one of the cognitive deficits that characterizes Alzheimer’s disease. Despite its prevalence and relevance to diagnosing Alzheimer’s disease, this topic has received little attention and is without comprehensive review. The review herein is aimed to fill this gap by first presenting an overview of the impairment caused in different clinical situations: pantomime of tool use, single tool use, real tool use, mechanical problem solving, function and manipulation knowledge tasks, and symbolic/meaningless gestures. On the basis of these results, we then propose alternative interpretations regarding the nature of the underlying mechanisms impaired by the disease. Also presented are principal methodological issues precluding firm conclusions from being drawn.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In the references, the studies preceded by (*), are those for which the data could have been transformed in percentage of accuracy for patients and controls. The results of these studies can be seen in the tables and the figures of this review. The studies preceded by (•) do not display data for controls (e.g., study of 1 case) and consequently the results can solely be seen in the tables of this review. The methods and type of data analyzed in the studies preceded by (♦) did not allow us to convert the performances of patients and controls in accuracy percentages (Benke et al. 1993; Parakh et al. 2004). The results of those studies do not appear in any figure or table but are discussed in the text. Finally, studies preceded by (°) are mentioned but cannot be discussed in the present review either because they are beyond the scope of this review (i.e., Jacobs et al. 1999) or because of methodological problems (Della Sala et al. 1987; Lucchelli et al., 1987; both symbolic and meaningless gestures were used for the imitation task; Giovannetti et al. 2006b: two of the 16 patients included in the study did not meet criteria for AD and were ultimately diagnosed with mixed dementia).

  2. As suggested elsewhere (e.g., Osiurak et al. 2010, 2011), we use the term “tool” for the implement performing an action (e.g., a hammer or a screwdriver) and “object” for the recipient of the action (e.g., a nail or a screw).

  3. We also used other procedures such as the ratio patients’/controls’ performance, but the results did not differ whatever the procedure used.

  4. Ceiling effects are very frequent in the field of apraxia even in patients with other neurological disorders such as vascular brain damage (e.g., De Renzi and Lucchelli 1988; Goldenberg and Hagmann 1998).

  5. This difference was significant for the study of Parakh et al. (2004) while this information was not available in the study of Rousseaux et al. (2012). Note also that the results of Parakh et al. (2004) are not shown in Table 2 and Fig. 3 because they were expressed in z-scores and we could not obtain the raw data.

  6. However, it is important to remind that we did not include in the present review the results of studies on everyday actions such as the naturalistic action test (e.g., Giovannetti et al. 2006a). More particularly, as mentioned above, executive functions are highly involved in this kind of tasks and, consequently, the interpretation of the results observed in these tasks are beyond the scope of the present review. Note also that we did not report the results of Benke (1993) because the data could not be transformed into percentages.

  7. The four sub-groups of Ochipa et al. (1992) were formed on the basis of the difficulties in terms of ideomotor praxis and semantic language impairment. Nevertheless, it is very likely that the different sub-groups also correspond to patients at different stages of the disease. So given that we could not obtain this information, we considered that the different sub-groups corresponded with patients at different stages of the disease.

References

  • *Adlam, A.L.R., Bozeat, S., Arnold, R., Watson, P., Hodges, J.R. (2006). Semantic knowledge in mild cognitive impairment and mild Alzheimer’s disease. Cortex, 42, 675–684.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartolo, A., Daumüller, M., Della Sala, S., & Goldenberg, G. (2007). Relationship between object-related gestures and the fractionated object knowledge system. Behavioural Neurology, 18, 143–147.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • ♦Benke, T. (1993). Two forms of apraxia in Alzheimer’s disease. Cortex, 29, 715–725.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benson, D. F., Davis, R. J., & Snyder, B. D. (1988). Posterior cortical atrophy. Archives of Neurology, 45, 789–793.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • *Blondel, A., Desgranges, B., de la Sayette, V., Schaeffer, S., Benali, K., Lechevalier, et al. (2001). Disorders in intentional gestural organization in Alzheimer’s disease: combined or selective impairment of the conceptual and production systems ? European Journal of Neurology, 8, 629–641.

  • Bozeat, S., Lambon Ralph, M. A., Patterson, K., & Hodges, J. R. (2002a). When objects lose their meaning: What happens to their use? Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neurosciences, 2, 236–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bozeat, S., Ralph, M. A., Patterson, K., & Hodges, J. R. (2002b). The influence of personal familiarity and context on object use in semantic dementia. Neurocase, 8, 127–134.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buxbaum, L. J. (2001). Ideomotor apraxia: a call to action. Neurocase, 7, 445–448.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Buxbaum, L. J., Giovannetti, T., & Libon, D. (2000a). The role of the dynamic body schema in praxis: Evidence from primary progressive apraxia. Brain and Cognition, 44, 166–191.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Buxbaum, L. J., Kyle, K. M., Grossman, M., & Coslett, H. B. (2007). Left inferior parietal representations for skilled hand-object interactions: evidence from stroke and corticobasal degeneration. Cortex, 43, 411–423.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buxbaum, L. J., Kyle, K. M., & Menon, R. (2005). On beyond mirror neurons: internal representations subserving imitation and recognition of skilled object-related actions in humans. Cognitive Brain Research, 25, 226–239.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buxbaum, L. J., & Saffran, E. M. (2002). Knowledge of object manipulation and object function: dissociations in apraxic and nonapraxic subjects. Brain and Language, 82, 179–199.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buxbaum, L. J., Schwartz, M. F., & Carew, T. G. (1997). The role of memory in object use. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 14, 219–254.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buxbaum, L. J., Veramonti, T., & Schwartz, M. F. (2000b). Function and manipulation tool knowledge in apraxia: knowing “what for” but not “how”. Neurocase, 6, 83–97.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chainay, H., Louarn, C., & Humphreys, G. W. (2006). Ideational action impairments in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain and Cognition, 62, 198–205.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • *Crutch, S.J., Rossor, M.N., Warrington, E.K. (2007). The quantitative assessment of apraxic deficits in Alzheimer’s disease. Cortex, 43, 976–986.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cubelli, R., & Della Sala, S. (1996). The legacy of automatic/voluntary dissociation in apraxia. Neurocase, 2, 449–454.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cubelli, R., Marchetti, C., Boscolo, G., & Della Sala, S. (2000). Cognition in action: testing a model of limb apraxia. Brain and Cognition, 44, 144–165.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • °Della Sala, S., Lucchelli, F., & Spinnler, H. (1987). Ideomotor apraxia in patients with dementia of Alzheimer type. Journal of Neurology, 234, 91–93.

  • De Renzi, E. (1989). Apraxia. In F. Boller & J. Grafman (Eds.), Handbook of neuropsychology (pp. 245–263). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Renzi, E., Faglioni, P., & Sorgato, P. (1982). Modality-specific and supramodal mechanisms of apraxia. Brain, 101, 301–312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Renzi, E., & Lucchelli, F. (1988). Ideational apraxia. Brain, 111, 1173–1185.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Vignemont, F. (2009). Body schema and body image. Neuropsychologia, 48, 669–680.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Derouesné, C., Lagha-Pierucci, S., Thibault, S., Baudoin-Madec, V., & Lacomblez, L. (2000). Neuropsychologia, 38, 1760–1769.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Donkervoort, M., Dekker, J., & Deelman, B. G. (2002). Sensitivity of different ADL measures to apraxia and motor impairments. Clinical Rehabilitation, 16, 299–305.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dubois, B., Feldman, H. H., Jacova, C., Cummings, J. L., DeKosky, S. T., Barbeger-Gateau, P., et al. (2010). Revising the definition of Alzheimer’s disease: a new lexicon. The Lancet Neurology, 9, 1118–1127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dubois, B., Feldman, H. H., Jacova, C., DeKosky, S. T., Barbeger-Gateau, P., et al. (2007). Research criteria for the diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: revising the NINCDS-ADRDA criteria. The Lancet Neurology, 6, 734–746.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Dumont, C., Ska, B., & Joanette, Y. (2000). Conceptual apraxia and semantic memory deficit in Alzheimer’s disease: two sides of the same coin ? Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6, 693–703.

    Google Scholar 

  • ♦Edwards, D.F., Deuel, R.K., Baum, C.M., Morris, J.C. (1991). A quantitative analysis of apraxia in senile dementia of the Alzheimer type: stage-related differences in prevalence and type. Dementia, 2, 142–149.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forde, E. M. E., & Humphreys, G. W. (2000). The role of semantic knowledge and working memory in everyday tasks. Brain and Cognition, 44, 214–252.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • *Foundas, A.L., Macauley, B.L., Raymer, A.M., Maher, L.M., Rothi, L.J.G., Heilman, K. M. (1999). Ideomotor apraxia in Alzheimer’s disease and left hemispheric stroke: limb transitive and intransitive movements. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and behavioral Neurology, 12, 161–166.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geschwind, N. (1965). Disconnection syndrome in animal and man. Brain, 88, 585–644.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Giannakopoulos, P., Duc, M., Gold, G., Hof, P. R., Michel, J.-P., & Bouras, C. (1998). Pathologic correlates of apraxia in Alzheimer disease. Archive of Neurology, 55, 689–695.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Giovannetti, T., Schmidt, K. S., Gallo, J. L., Sestito, N., & Libon, D. J. (2006). Everyday action in dementia: evidence for differential deficits in Alzheimer’s disease versus subcortical vascular dementia. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 12, 45–53.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • °Giovannetti, T., Sestito, N., Libon, D.J., Schmidt, K.S., Gallo, J.L., Gambino, et al. (2006b). The influence of personal familiarity on object naming, knowledge, and use in dementia. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 21, 607–614.

  • Goldenberg, G. (1995). Imitating gestures and manipulating a manikin – The representation of the human body in ideomotor apraxia. Neuropsychologia, 33, 63–72.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Goldenberg, G. (1999). Matching and imitation of hand and finger postures in patients with damage in the left or right hemispheres. Neuropsychologia, 37, 559–566.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Goldenberg, G. (2003). Apraxia and beyond: life and work of Hugo Liepmann. Cortex, 39, 509–524.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldenberg, G. (2009). Apraxia and the parietal lobes. Neuropsychologia, 47, 1449–1559.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldenberg, G., & Hagmann, S. (1997). The meaning of meaningless gestures: a study of visuo-imitative apraxia. Neuropsychologia, 35, 333–341.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Goldenberg, G., & Hagmann, S. (1998). Tool use and mechanical problem solving in apraxia. Neuropsychologia, 36, 581–589.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Goldenberg, G., Hentze, S., & Hermsdörfer, J. (2004). The effect of tactile feedback on pantomime of tool use in apraxia. Neurology, 63, 1863–1867.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Goldenberg, G., & Spatt, J. (2009). The neural basis of tool use. Brain, 132, 1645–1655.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Graham, N. L., Zeman, A., Young, A. W., Patterson, K., & Hodges, J. R. (1999). Dyspraxia in a patient with corticobasal degeneration: the role of visual and tactile inputs to action. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 67, 334–344.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Green, R. C., Goldstein, F. C., Mira, S. S., Alazraki, N. P., Baxt, J. L., & Bakay, R. A. (1995). Slowly progressive apraxia in Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 59, 312–315.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Harley, T. A. (2004). Does cognitive neuropsychology have a future ? Cognitive Neuropsychology, 21, 3–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hartmann, K., Goldenberg, G., Daumüller, M., & Hermsdörfer, J. (2005). It takes the whole brain to make a cup of coffee: the neuropsychology of naturalistic actions involving technical devices. Neuropsychologia, 43, 625–627.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heilman, K. M., Rothi, L. J. G., Mack, L., Feinberg, T., & Watson, R. T. (1986). Apraxia after superior parietal lesions. Cortex, 32, 141–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heilman, K. M., Rothi, L. J., & Valenstein, E. (1982). Two forms of ideomotor apraxia. Neurology, 22, 342–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heilman, K. M., & Watson, R. T. (2008). The disconnection apraxias. Cortex, 44, 975–982.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hermsdörfer, J., Hentze, S., & Goldenberg, G. (2006). Spatial and kinematic features of apraxic movement depend on the mode of execution. Neuropsychologia, 44, 1642–1652.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hodges, J. R., Bozeat, S., Lambon Ralph, M. A., Patterson, K., & Spatt, J. (2000). The role of knowledge in object use: evidence from semantic dementia. Brain, 123, 1913–1925.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hodges, J. R., Spatt, J., & Patterson, K. (1999). What” and “how”: evidence for the dissociation of object knowledge and mechanical problem-solving skills in the human brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 96, 9444–9448.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • °Jacobs, D.H., Adair, J.C., Williamson, D.J.G., Na, D.L., Gold, M., Foundas, A.L., et al. (1999). Apraxia and motor-skill acquisition in Alzheimer’s disease are dissociable. Neuropsychologia, 37, 875–880.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jarry, C., Osiurak, F., Delafuys, D., Chauviré, V., Etcharry-Bouyx, F., Le Gall, D. (2013). Apraxia of tool use: more evidence for the technical reasoning hypothesis. Cortex. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2013.02.011.

  • *Kato, M., Meguro, K., Sato, M., Shimada, Y., Yamazaki, H., Saito, H., et al. (2000). Ideomotor apraxia in patients with Alzheimer’s disease: why do they use their body parts as objects? Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and behavioral Neurology, 14, 45–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirk, A., & Kertesz, A. (1991). On drawing impairment in Alzheimer’s disease. Archives of Neurology, 48, 73–77.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lauro-Grotto, R., Piccini, C., & Shallice, T. (1997). Modality-specific operations in semantic dementia. Cortex, 33, 593–622.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • °Lucchelli, F., Lopez, O.L., Faglioni, P., Boller, F. (1993). Ideomotor and ideational apraxia in Alzheimer’s disease. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 8, 413–417.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luria, A. R., & Tsvetkova, L. S. (1964). The programming of constructive activity in local brain injuries. Neuropsychologia, 2, 95–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McKhann, G., Drachman, D., Folstein, M., Katzman, R., Price, D., & Stadlan, E. M. (1984). Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology, 34, 939–944.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mesulam, M. M. (2003). Primary progressive aphasia – A language based dementia. The New England Journal of Medicine, 349, 1535–1542.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • •Moreaud, O., Charnallet, A., Pellat, J. (1998). Identification without manipulation: a study of the relations between object use and semantic memory. Neuropsychologia, 36, 1295–1301.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Mozaz, M., Garaigorbodil, M., Rothi, L. J. G., Anderson, J., Crucian, G. P., Heilman, K. M. (2006). Posture recognition in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain and Cognition, 62, 241–245.

    Google Scholar 

  • •Negri, G.A., Lunardelli, A., Reverberi, C., Gigli, G.L., Rumiati, R.I. (2007). Degraded semantic knowledge and accurate object use. Cortex, 43, 376–388.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ochipa, C., Rothi, L. J. G., & Heilman, K. M. (1989). Ideational apraxia: a deficit in tool selection and use. Annals of Neurology, 25, 190–193.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Ochipa, C., Rothi, L.J. G., Heilman, K.M. (1992). Conceptual apraxia in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain, 115, 1061–1071.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Okazaki, M., Kasai, M., Meguro, K., Yamaguchi, S., Ishii, H. (2009). Disturbances in everyday life activities and sequence disabilities in tool use for Alzheimer disease and vascular dementia. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 22, 215–221.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osiurak, F., Aubin, G., Allain, P., Jarry, C., Richard, I., & Le Gall, D. (2008). Object usage and object utilization. A single-case study. Neurocase, 14, 169–183.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Osiurak, F., Jarry, C., Allain, P., Aubin, G., Etcharry-Bouyx, F., Richard, I., et al. (2007). Des troubles praxiques aux troubles techniques. Une étude de deux cas. Revue de Neuropsychologie, 17, 231–259.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osiurak, F., Jarry, C., Allain, P., Aubin, G., Etcharry-Bouyx, F., Richard, I., et al. (2009). Unusual use of objects after unilateral brain damage. The technical reasoning model. Cortex, 45, 769–783.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Osiurak, F., Jarry, C., & Le Gall, D. (2010). Grasping the affordances, understanding the reasoning. Toward a dialectical theory of human tool use. Psychological Review, 117, 517–540.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Osiurak, F., Jarry, C., & Le Gall, D. (2011). Re-examining the gesture engram hypothesis. New perspectives on apraxia of tool use. Neuropsychologia, 49, 299–312.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Osiurak, F., Jarry, C., Lesourd, M., Baumard, J., Le Gall, D. (2013). Mechanical problem-solving strategies in left-brain damaged patients and apraxia of tool use. Neuropsychologia, 51, 1964–1972.

  • Osiurak, F., & Le Gall, D. (2012). Apraxia: clinical types, theoretical models, and evaluation. In T. Heinbockel (Ed.), Neuroscience (pp. 69–92). InTech.

  • ♦Parakh, R., Roy, E., Koo, E., Black, S. (2004). Pantomime and imitation of limb gestures in relation to the severity of Alzheimer’s disease. Brain and Cognition, 55, 272–274.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Rapcsak, S. Z., Croswell, S. C., Rubens, A. B. (1989). Apraxia in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology, 39, 664–668.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rothi, L. J. G., Ochipa, C., & Heilman, K. M. (1991). A cognitive neuropsychological model of limb praxis. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 8, 443–458.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Rousseaux, M., Rénier, J., Anicet, L., Pasquier, F., Mackowiak-Cordoliani, M.A. (2012). Gesture comprehension, knowledge and production in Alzheimer’s disease. European Journal of Neurology, 19, 1037–1044.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roy, E. A., & Square, P. A. (1985). Common considerations in the study of limb, verbal and oral apraxia. In E. A. Roy (Ed.), Neuropsychological studies of apraxia and related disorders (pp. 111–161). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salter, J. E., Roy, E. A., Black, S. E., Joshi, A., & Almeida, Q. (2004). Gestural imitation and limb apraxia in corticobasal degeneration. Brain and Cognition, 55, 400–402.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Schwartz, R.L., Adair, J.C., Raymer, A.M., Williamson, D.J.G., Crosson, B., Rothi, L.J.G., et al. (2000). Conceptual apraxia in probable Alzheimer’s disease as demonstrated by the Florida Action Recall Test. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6, 265–270.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silveri, M. C., & Ciccarelli, N. (2009). Semantic memory in object use. Neuropsychologia, 47, 2634–2641.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sirigu, A., Duhamel, J.-R., & Poncet, M. (1991). The role of sensorimotor experience in object recognition. Brain, 114, 2555–2573.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sperling, R. A., Aisen, P. S., Beckett, L. A., Bennett, D. A., Craft, S., Fagan, A. M., et al. (2011). Toward defining the preclinical stages of Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 7, 280–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stopford, C. L., Snowden, J. S., Thompson, J. C., & Neary, D. (2008). Variability in cognitive presentation of Alzheimer’s disease. Cortex, 44, 185–195.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tessari, A., Canessa, N., Ukmar, M., & Rumiati, R. I. (2007). Neuropsychological evidence for a strategic control of multiple routes in imitation. Brain, 130, 1111–1126.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Travniczek-Marterer, A., Danielczyk, W., Simanyi, M., Fischer, P. (1993). Ideomotor apraxia in Alzheimer’s disease. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 88, 1–4.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E. (1972). Episodic and semantic memory. In E. Tulving & W. Donaldson (Eds.), Organization of memory (pp. 381–403). London: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wada, Y., Nakagawa, Y., Nishikawa, T., Aso, N., Inokowa, M., Kashiwagi, A., et al. (1999). Role of somatosensory feedback from tools in realizing movements by patients with ideomotor apraxia. European Neurology, 41, 73–78.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Walther, S., Vanbellingen, T., Muri, R., Strik, W., & Bohlalter, S. (2013). Impaired pantomime in schizophrenia: association with frontal lobe function. Cortex, 49, 520–527.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warren, M. (1981). Relationship of constructional apraxia and body scheme disorders to dressing performance in adult CVA. The American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 7, 431–437.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Willis, L., Behrens, M., Mack, W., Chui, H. (1998). Ideomotor apraxia in early Alzheimer’s disease: time and accuracy measures. Brain and Cognition, 38, 220–233.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yamaguchi, H., Maki, Y., & Yamagami, T. (2010). Yamaguchi fox-pigeon imitation test: a rapid test for dementia. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 29, 254–258.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by grants from ANR (Agence Nationale pour la Recherche; Project Démences et Utilisation d’Outils/Dementia and Tool Use, N°ANR 2011 MALZ 006 03) to François Osiurak and Didier Le Gall, Région Pays de la Loire (Project Outils et Vie Quotidienne/Tool Use and Daily Life Activities, 2012-09689 OVQ) to Didier Le Gall, and was performed within the framework of the LABEX CORTEX (ANR-11-LABX-0042) of Université de Lyon (François Osiurak), within the program “Investissements d’Avenir” (ANR-11-IDEX-0007) operated by the French National Research Agency (ANR).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Mathieu Lesourd or François Osiurak.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Lesourd, M., Le Gall, D., Baumard, J. et al. Apraxia and Alzheimer’s Disease: Review and Perspectives. Neuropsychol Rev 23, 234–256 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-013-9235-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-013-9235-4

Keywords

Navigation